Within 8 weeks of corticosteroid administration, a diagnosis of CIPD was made for 14 (10.1%) of 139 episodes in 135 patients with a non-CNS-SLE flare. Using multiple logistic regression analysis, we identified positive Q(albumin) (CSF/serum albumin ratio; an indicator of blood-brain barrier [BBB] damage) (odds ratio [OR], 33.3; 95% confidence interval [CI], 3.64-304; p = 0.002) and low serum levels of complements (OR, 0.91; 95% CI, 0.83-1.00; p = 0.047) as independent risk factors for CIPDs. Positive Qalbumin was detected in 45% (5 of 11) of episodes in which CIPDs developed. Compared with episodes in which no psychiatric events occurred, a higher level
of Qalbumin this website was found in episodes in which CIPDs developed, and an even higher level was noted in episodes with active CNS-SLE (Jonckheere-Terpstra test, p<0.001). Although no causal links have been proven, the results from the present study raise the possibility that BBB damage may be associated with SLE- and corticosteroid-induced behavioral changes. (C) 2007 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.”
“A crucial question in cognitive science is how linguistic and visual information are integrated. Previous research has shown that eye movements to objects in the visual environment are locked to linguistic input. More surprisingly, listeners fixate on now-empty regions that
had previously been occupied by relevant
objects. This ‘looking at nothing’ phenomenon has been Selleck GSK621 linked to the claim that the visual system constructs sparse representations of the external world and relies on saccades and fixations to extract information in a just-in-time manner. Our model provides a different explanation: based on recent work in visual Depsipeptide mw cognition and memory, it assumes that the visual system creates and stores detailed internal memory representations, and that looking at nothing facilitates retrieval of those representations.”
“Objectives. To evaluate whether social contacts, support, and social strain/conflict are related to executive function and memory abilities in middle-age and older adults.
Methods. Longitudinal data on social contacts, support, and strain/conflict were examined in relation to executive function and memory at ages 35-85 years using data from the national Midlife in the U.S. (MIDUS) study. Age-related differences in patterns of association were also examined.
Results. Regression analyses, controlling for age, sex, race, education, chronic health conditions, and health behaviors, revealed significant positive associations between histories of greater social contacts and support and both executive function and episodic memory, whereas declines in social contacts were negatively associated with both outcomes.