To quantify these

effects, we fit the data for each obser

To quantify these

effects, we fit the data for each observer with Naka-Rushton functions (Herrmann et al., 2010; Pestilli et al., 2009; Naka and Rushton, 1966; Ling et al., 2010), for which two key parameters are predicted to change under the normalization framework: C50 and d′max. These parameters have been used in previous psychophysics studies as metrics for changes in contrast Hydroxychloroquine gain and response gain. The C50 parameter corresponds to the semi-saturation constant, and changes in this parameter with rivalry suppression indicate a contrast gain shift. The d′max parameter corresponds to the asymptotic response at high contrasts, and changes in this parameter indicate a response gain reduction. Parameter estimates revealed a pattern consistent with predictions of the normalization model of attention: C50 shifted toward higher contrasts for dominant stimuli regardless of their size, whereas d′max was attenuated the most when the dominant stimulus was the same size as the probe stimulus. Consistent GSK1210151A datasheet with these results, response gain-like modulation has previously been found with rivalry when similar-sized stimuli are pit against each other, both in single-unit (Sengpiel and Blakemore, 1994) and behavioral studies

(Ling et al., 2010; Watanabe et al., 2004). Fitting the data separately for each individual yielded a similar pattern of results (Figures 3B and 3C; Figure S1 available online). When the dominant stimulus was large, there was solely a change in C50 for all observers (Figure 3B), with no change in d′max (Figure 3C). However, as the size

of the competitor approached that of the probe, changes in both C50 and d′max emerged. While standard normalization models would only predict a contrast gain shift (Moradi and Heeger, 2009), our results indicate that an additional Dextrose mechanism is needed to account for our results; indeed, the conjoint reduction in both contrast gain (C50) and response gain (d′max) when the dominant stimulus is small is a prediction borne from the normalization model of attention for scenarios where the probe is small and the modulatory field is roughly the same size (Reynolds and Heeger, 2009). One alternative explanation for the large competitor’s inability to suppress high contrast probes is center-surround interactions that plausibly could weaken the strength of the center region of the competing stimulus. Although center-surround inhibition has been shown to be least effective in the fovea (Petrov et al., 2005), the retinal region targeted by our stimuli, we sought to rule out this alternative explanation explicitly by performing an additional control experiment, where we measured the degree to which the surround region of the large stimulus attenuated its center portion (Figure S2).

035; Figures 5B, right, and 5D) Thus, this first set of experime

035; Figures 5B, right, and 5D). Thus, this first set of experiments seems to rule out a role of the selleckchem intracellular region, ion channel, and the ATD of GluK3 and points to the LBD as a potential zinc binding domain responsible for the facilitatory effect of zinc. The LBD is formed by two extracellular segments referred to as S1 and S2 (Stern-Bach et al., 1994), which form a clamshell-like structure where S1 forms most of

the upper half of the clamshell, and S2 forms most of the lower half. We next tested which of these segments is involved in the facilitatory effect of zinc on GluK3 receptors by using chimeric GluK2 and GluK3 receptors where S2 of one is replaced by the other and vice versa. Interestingly, currents mediated by GluK3/GluK2 chimeric receptors that contain S2 and the intracellular part of the GluK2 subunit (GluK3/K2S2) were inhibited (54% ± 6%; n = 5; p = 0.002; Figure 5C, left, and Figure 5D), whereas currents mediated by GluK3/GluK2 this website chimeric receptors containing S2 and the intracellular part of the GluK3 subunit (GluK2/K3S2) were facilitated by 100 μM zinc (197% ± 9%, n = 5; p = 0.034; Figure 5C, right, and Figure 5D). Moreover, whereas desensitization kinetics in control conditions was not affected for most of the constructs tested (Figure 5E), the kinetics of GluK3/K2S2 and GluK2/K3S2 was considerably changed (from 5.0 ± 0.2 ms, n = 8 for WT GluK3 to 14.5 ± 0.8 ms, n = 4, p < 0.0001 for GluK3/K2S2;

and from 3.4 ± 0.1 ms, n = 5 for WT GluK2 to 2 ± 0.1 ms for GluK2/K3S2, n = 4, p < 0.0001). Slowed desensitization kinetics could explain why GluK3 with the S2 segment substituted for GluK2 is functional as assessed

by slow glutamate all application on Xenopus oocytes ( Strutz et al., 2001). These experiments clearly point to the S2 segment of GluK3 as a target for zinc binding. To further characterize the zinc binding site, we hypothesized that it might stabilize the interface between LBDs by binding to a unique site generated by amino acids found only in GluK3. Among residues that usually bind zinc (histidine, cysteine, aspartate, and glutamate), a single residue in S2 differs between GluK3 and the other KARs: An aspartate in GluK3 (D759) is replaced by a glycine in GluK1 and GluK2 and by an asparagine in GluK4 and GluK5 (Figure 6A). We tested the effects of zinc (100 μM) on the reciprocal mutants GluK3(D759G) and GluK2(G758D). Glutamate-activated currents were potentiated by zinc in GluK2(G758D) receptors to the same extent as GluK3 (177% ± 7% of control amplitude, n = 6; p = 0.008; Figures 6B and 6F). Conversely, GluK3(D759G) currents were inhibited by zinc (32% ± 9%, n = 5; Figures 6C and 6F). These results clearly indicate that the replacement of G758 in GluK2 by an aspartate is sufficient to confer zinc potentiation in GluK2. Moreover, desensitization was markedly slower in GluK3(D759G) (τdes = 18.4 ± 1.8 ms; n = 8; p < 0.0001) and greatly accelerated in GluK2(G758D) (τdes = 1.3 ± 0.1 ms; n = 6; p < 0.

The changes of membrane potentials in response to two opposing di

The changes of membrane potentials in response to two opposing directions of FM sweeps were recorded under the current-clamp mode (Figure 3A). By examining the cell’s membrane potential changes evoked by FM sweeps at various speeds, we determined the DSI of membrane potential changes for the recorded neuron (Figure 3B). For this neuron, selleck upward direction was defined as the preferred direction for FM sweeps, because it evoked large depolarization of the cell’s membrane potential, whereas

downward direction was assigned as the null direction, because it generated large hyperpolarization. The DSI of the membrane potential change for this neuron was greatest for a sweep speed of 70 octaves/s. Ulixertinib Note that for the following high-quality voltage-clamp recordings, spikes of the recorded neuron were blocked due to QX-314, which was included in the intracellular solution. Previous studies demonstrated that the subthreshold responses and their DSIs under such circumstances were highly correlated with spike responses and their DSI (Wu et al., 2008 and Ye et al., 2010). Thus, the direction selectivity of those recorded neurons under our experimental conditions can

be represented by the subthreshold membrane potential responses with reasonable fidelity. After switching to the voltage-clamp mode, excitatory inputs were measured by clamping the neuron’s membrane potential at −70mV, the potential levels close to the reversal potential for GABAA receptors, whereas inhibitory inputs were recorded at 0mV holding potential, the reversal potential for glutamate receptors’ mediated currents (Figure 3C). In response to FM sweeps at the speed of 70 octaves/s, neither the excitatory nor the inhibitory inputs were direction selective, which suggests that the cell’s direction selectivity is not inherited from afferent inputs (Figure 3D). It implies that the direction selectivity of its membrane potential changes must be constructed within this cell. Linear current-voltage relationship (I-V curve) was observed Fluorouracil supplier for the recorded

synaptic currents evoked by the CF tones of the recorded neurons at 60 dB SPL (Figure 4B). The derived reversal potential for the early component of these currents (mainly excitatory) was 0 ± 6mV (SD), close to the known reversal potential for glutamatergic currents. These data suggest that under our voltage-clamp recording conditions, synaptic inputs that contributed to the recorded currents were detected with reasonable accuracy (see Experimental Procedures). Previous intracellular studies suggested that inhibition might play an important role in shaping direction selectivity of auditory neurons (Gittelman et al., 2009, Ye et al., 2010 and Zhang et al., 2003). To examine the interaction of synaptic excitation and inhibition, we derived excitatory and inhibitory conductance from recorded synaptic inputs (Figure 4A).

, 2006;

, 2006; Gamma-secretase inhibitor Koh et al., 2004; Marie et al., 2004; Verstreken et al., 2002; Wagh et al., 2006; Zinsmaier et al., 1994), but we find that these are not affected upon expression of PH-GRP1 (Figures S2A–S2E). Finally, we assessed the abundance of Syntaxin1A, a protein essential for synaptic vesicle fusion (Schulze et al., 1995) that enriches to PI(4,5)P2-containing microdomains in PC12 cells (Aoyagi et al., 2005; van den Bogaart et al., 2011). Expression of PH-GRP1 results in significantly less Syntaxin1A labeling at synaptic boutons (Figures 2A and 2B, blue). This effect is caused by reduced PI(3,4,5)P3 availability, as RNAi to PI3Kinase92E

also results in less Syntaxin1A labeling and coexpression of the PH-GRP1 probe together with the Lyn11-FRB/FKBP-p85 in the presence of rapamycin restores the Syntaxin1A labeling defect (Figures 2A and 2B, blue). In contrast, expression of the PLCδ1-PH probe that shields PI(4,5)P2 or RNAi to PI4P5Kinase does not significantly affect Syntaxin1A labeling intensity at neuromuscular boutons (Figures 2A and 2C). MI-773 The data suggest that Syntaxin1A levels in Drosophila third-instar bouton

membranes are more sensitive to PI(3,4,5)P3 availability than they are to PI(4,5)P2. PI(3,4,5)P3 localizes to presynaptic microdomains in the membrane, and to investigate whether Syntaxin1A also concentrates at these sites, we used superresolution PiMP and SR-SIM imaging. We find that in control boutons, Syntaxin1A is enriched in plasma membrane-bound domains (Figure 2D), and these domains extensively colocalize with the active zone marker RBP (Figures 2G and 2H). Interestingly, in boutons that express PH-GRP1, these Syntaxin1A domains are largely dispersed (Figures 2E and 2E′) and much less Syntaxin1A colocalizes with RBP (Figures 2G and 2I). Indicating that this defect is specific to reduced PI(3,4,5)P3 levels, Syntaxin1A in PH-GRP1, Lyn11-FRB/FKBP-p85-expressing

animals placed on rapamycin Protein kinase N1 now again concentrates in defined clusters that colocalize with split Venus-PH-GRP1 (Figures 2F and 2F′; 73% of the GRP1 puncta overlap with a Syntaxin1A spot) and with anti-RBP (Figure 2G). Thus, at Drosophila neuromuscular boutons, Syntaxin1A largely colocalizes with PI(3,4,5)P3 at active zones, and this Syntaxin1A localization is dependent on the presence of PI(3,4,5)P3. Finally, also in PC12 cell membrane sheets that we labeled using recombinant GRP1-PH-mCherry and anti-Syntaxin1A antibodies, we find extensive colocalization ( Figure S2F; 84% of the GRP1 puncta overlap with a Syntaxin1A spot), and this colocalization is more prevalent than when membrane sheets were labeled with PLCδ1-PH-GFP and anti-Syntaxin1A ( van den Bogaart et al., 2011). Hence, access to PI(3,4,5)P3 is necessary for the formation of normal Syntaxin1A domains in the membrane in vivo.

, 2003, Bowater et al , 2003 and Dubey, 2010), and seroprevalence

, 2003, Bowater et al., 2003 and Dubey, 2010), and seroprevalence in these animals in Atlantic click here and Pacific ocean dolphins is very high ( Dubey et al., 2003, Cabezón et al., 2004 and Forman et al., 2009). This high seroprevalence is intriguing because dolphins drink little water ( Dubey et al., 2003). To our knowledge there is only one report of toxoplasmosis in an adult tucuxi (Sotalia guianensis) from Rio de Janeiro, Brazil ( Bandoli and Oliveira, 1977). We report here for the first time prevalence of T. gondii antibodies in the Amazon River dolphin (Inia geoffrensis) or boto from

Central Amazon, Brazil. Blood samples were collected from 95 Amazon River dolphins of both genders and various ages, free-living in the Mamiraua (64°45′W, 03°35′S) during capture/release expeditions of the Projeto Boto from 2001 to 2003. The capture and collection protocols for biological material are described in da Silva and Martin (2000). Blood was obtained by venipuncture and the serum was kept at −20 °C until the completion of serological tests. Sera were assayed for antibodies to T. gondii by the modified agglutination test (MAT) as described selleck monoclonal humanized antibody inhibitor by Dubey and Desmonts (1987).

Sera were screened in 1:25, 1:50, and 1:500 dilutions, and positive and negative controls were included in each run. A titer of 1:25 was considered indicative of T. gondii infection ( Dubey et al., 2003 and Cabezón et al., 2004). For the statistical analysis of the variables gender (male and female) and age (young and adults) we used the

Chi-square (χ2) test with significance level at 5%, using the program EPI INFO version 3.5.1. ifoxetine Antibodies to T. gondii were found in 82 of 95 (86.3%) botos with titers of 1:25 in 24 (29.3%), 50 in 56 (68.3%), and 500 in 2 (2.4%). There was no significant variance with regard to gender (P = 0.93, 45 of 52 [86.5%] males were seropositive, and 37 of 42 [88.1%] females were seropositive) or age of dolphins (P = 0.6, 85.7% seropositivity in 14 young, 87.0% seropositivity in 87 adults). Sixty-one dolphins were sampled more than once during the period; 42 dolphins were positive in all samplings; 5 animals were negative in all samplings; 13 dolphins that were seronegative in the first collection became positive in subsequent samplings; and 1 dolphin with a low MAT titer of 1:25 became negative in subsequent sampling. The high prevalence T. gondii antibodies in healthy Amazon River dolphins in the present study indicates that the infection by this pathogen is frequent. One dolphin with a low titer of 1:25 was seronegative in the second sampling; this could be due to test variability or due to transient T. gondii infection. Waste from domestic and wild cats containing oocysts of T. gondii can be carried by the water from sewage, agricultural waste and rain polluting the rivers, estuaries, coastal areas and beaches ( Bowater et al., 2003).

We also performed an analysis of our data to confirm that the str

We also performed an analysis of our data to confirm that the striatal deactivation was not a physiological artifact (Figure S1C). Strikingly, the only brain region commonly active between the time of incentive presentation (Table S1) and the execution of the motor task (Table S2) was bilaterally encompassing ventral striatum (Table S3). Furthermore, additional whole brain analyses did not reveal any brain regions that were directly correlated with mTOR phosphorylation participants’ parabolic behavioral performance or interactions between incentive level and task difficulty (see Supplemental Information for details,

Figure S1E). These analyses provided us with further evidence of the ventral striatum’s integral role in mediating participants’ I-BET-762 concentration responses during performance for incentives. The idiosyncratic pattern of striatal activity we observed (i.e., activation at the time of incentive presentation and deactivation at the time of action) resembles that reported for participants experiencing potential monetary gains and losses (Tom et al., 2007 and Yacubian et al., 2006). Tom et al. (2007) found that ventral striatum was activated by the prospect of gains, and deactivated by the prospect of losses, and that such deactivation was strongly correlated with a behavioral measure of loss aversion.

The findings of Tom et al. (2007), in conjunction with our results, led us to develop a new hypothesis regarding the role of ventral striatum in mediating performance decrements for large incentives: deactivation of ventral striatum during motor action reflects evaluation

of the potential loss (of a presumed gain) that would arise from failure to successfully achieve the task. Essentially, larger incentives are framed as larger potential losses, and as these perceived potential losses increase (in the highest incentive conditions) they are manifested as performance decrements. Because this hypothesis is generated in part from a “reverse-inference” (Poldrack, 2006), we needed to obtain additional evidence in order to provide direct empirical support. Our hypothesis led to the following Oxaliplatin predictions: (1) striatal deactivation at the time of motor action would predict the extent of individuals’ decrements in behavioral performance; (2) activity in ventral striatum during motor action would relate to an individual’s behavioral loss aversion (i.e., the more loss averse a participant, the greater her ventral striatal deactivation during motor action); and (3) a participant’s degree of behavioral loss aversion would be predictive of her propensity to exhibit performance decrements for large incentives, as well as the level of incentive that resulted in peak performance. To test the first prediction, we examined the extent to which a participant’s decrease in performance at the highest incentive level was related to her neural sensitivity to incentive.

To verify that loss of IRs in the outer dendrite in IR8a mutants

To verify that loss of IRs in the outer dendrite in IR8a mutants was not simply due to a failure in formation of this sensory compartment, we expressed a GFP-tagged tubulin isoform (GFP:α1tub84B) in these neurons, which serves as a robust reporter of the outer segment in ciliated sensory neurons in Drosophila ( Avidor-Reiss et al., 2004). In wild-type and IR8a mutant neurons, GFP:α1tub84B displayed a similar distribution distal to 21A6 in Lonafarnib mouse both coeloconic sensilla in the main body of the antenna and grooved peg sensilla in

the sacculus ( Figures 3B and S2B). Thus, the outer ciliated segment forms correctly in IR8a mutants, supporting a specific role for this receptor in targeting odor-specific IRs to this cellular compartment. We investigated whether there was a reciprocal requirement for IR84a for the localization of IR8a by expressing EGFP:IR8a in IR84a mutant neurons ( Figure 3C). The normal cilia distribution of this fusion protein is severely impaired in

the absence of IR84a ( Figure 3C). Similarly, IR8a cilia localization is abolished in IR64a mutant sacculus neurons ( Figure S2C). In both mutant backgrounds, IR8a localization is restored by the expression of corresponding IR rescue transgenes ( Figures 3C and S2C). Thus, selleckchem efficient cilia targeting of IR8a depends upon the presence of an odor-specific partner. Having shown that phenylacetaldehyde responses in ac4 sensilla require two receptors, IR84a and IR8a (Figures 2B and 2C) (Y. Grosjean and R. B., unpublished data), we asked whether these proteins are sufficient for the reconstitution of a functional olfactory receptor in heterologous neurons. We previously showed that ectopic expression of IR84a in ac3 neurons is sufficient to confer responsiveness to phenylacetaldehyde (Benton et al., 2009). However,

IR8a secondly is also expressed endogenously in these cells (data not shown), raising the possibility that the odor-evoked responses are not due to IR84a alone, but depend on IR84a in combination with IR8a. To resolve this issue, we expressed IR84a in OR22a neurons, which innervate basiconic sensilla and do not express IR8a (Figure 1C). When expressed alone in these neurons, EGFP:IR84a fails to localize to sensory cilia, where OR22a concentrates (Figure 4A) (Dobritsa et al., 2003). However, when EGFP:IR84a is coexpressed with IR8a, the fusion protein is efficiently transported to the ciliated sensory endings (Figure 4A). As in coeloconic sensilla, we observed a reciprocal requirement for IR84a in the cilia localization of IR8a in OR neurons: alone, EGFP:IR8a was absent from sensory cilia, but coexpression of IR84a was sufficient to promote its redistribution to the sensory compartment (Figure 4A). We examined the functionality of these cilia-localized receptors by electrophysiological analysis of phenylacetaldehyde-evoked responses. OR22a neurons expressing EGFP:IR84a or EGFP:IR8a alone do not respond to this odor above basal solvent-evoked activity.

Fourth, most of the enclosed dendrites are in the medial-lateral

Fourth, most of the enclosed dendrites are in the medial-lateral (or dorsal-ventral) orientation. With the exception of the enclosed dendrites, most class IV da dendrites are thus located in a 2D sheet at the interface of the epidermal basal surface and the ECM. We then performed time-lapse analyses of how epidermal cells enclose dendrites by imaging the ventral dendritic field of the same ddaC neurons at 72 hr after egg laying (AEL) (Figure 2A) and 84 hr selleck chemical AEL (Figure 2B) and

comparing the distribution of enclosed dendrites. Newly enclosed dendrites were found to emerge in three different ways. First, stabilized branches initially attached to the ECM can subsequently

become enclosed (arrowheads). Second, an enclosed dendrite tip can continue to grow within the epidermal layer (arrows). Third, a dendrite tip that was attached to the ECM can extend a new segment into the epidermal layer (open arrowheads). How do existing and new dendrites grow into the epidermal layer? The first scenario can result from the basal plasma membrane of epidermal cells wrapping around an existing dendritic branch. The second and third scenarios indicate that dendrite tips can grow inside the epidermal layer either by “burrowing a tunnel” or by pushing through spaces between cells. To test these hypotheses, we further examined the spatial relationship of class IV da dendrites TSA HDAC research buy and the epidermis by transmission electron microscopy (TEM). In order to unequivocally identify the neuronal structures of interest, we used two pre-embedding staining strategies to specifically label the dendrites of class IV da neurons. The first strategy involved antibody staining against RFP in ppk-CD4-tdTom animals, with subsequent

HRP-conjugated secondary antibody labeling. The second strategy was to express a membrane-tethered HRP transgene, UAS-HRP-DsRed-GPI, in class IV da neurons with an improved ppk-Gal4 that has higher and more specific expression (see Experimental Procedures). In both cases, the HRP reaction product Diaminobenzidine (DAB) can be detected by TEM ( Larsen et al., 2003). Our TEM analysis revealed that, whereas most dendrites are for located underneath the basal surface of epidermal cells and are in direct contact with the ECM ( Figures 2D–2F), there are three types of dendrite enclosure in the epidermal layer. First, we observed thick enclosed dendrites connected to the ECM through a channel formed by opposing epidermal cell membranes ( Figure 2G), suggesting the wrapping of existing dendrites by the epidermal basal surface. Second, some dendrites are located between cell junctions of neighboring epidermal cells ( Figure 2H), confirming that dendrites can indeed grow between epidermal cells.

A K from the Esther A & Joseph Klingenstein Foundation, the Edw

A.K. from the Esther A. & Joseph Klingenstein Foundation, the Edward Mallinckrodt, 3-Methyladenine concentration Jr. Foundation, the Whitehall Foundation, and the Alzheimer’s Association. We thank Dr. G. Danuzer and Dr. K. Jaqaman for kindly sharing their uTrack particle tracking software. We also thank Dr. S. Mennerick, Dr. V. Cavalli, and Dr. D. Owyoung for their

constructive comments on the manuscript. “
“Over the course of development, numerous molecules are repurposed to function in distinct cellular contexts (Charron and Tessier-Lavigne, 2007). During the earliest phases of neural development the Hedgehog signaling pathway plays an important role establishing patterning of the central nervous system. (Ericson et al., 1995, Roelink et al., 1995, Xu et al., 2005 and Xu et al., 2010). The secreted protein Sonic Hedgehog (Shh) is expressed in the notochord and floor plate of the neural tube and, cells adopt

specific fates based upon their level of exposure to the established Shh gradient. At later stages, during development of the telencephalon, Sonic Hedgehog adopts a similar function where it is expressed in the ventral telencephalon and functions to maintain ventral identity through its regulation of expression of the transcription factor Nkx2.1 (Xu et al., 2010). Shh is also expressed in adult neural stem cell niches where it helps maintain adult neural stem cell identity (Machold et al., 2003 and Palma et al., 2005). Cell fate specification by Shh is regulated through the canonical learn more Shh signaling pathway whereby binding the Shh receptor Patched (Ptc) relieves inhibition of the transmembrane protein Smoothened (Smo) (Rohatgi et al., 2007). Smoothened signaling leads to the activation of see more the Gli family of transcription factors, which mediates the cell fate specification functions of Shh (Ahn and Joyner, 2005 and Palma et al., 2005). Later in development, after the tissues have been specified, Shh expressed from the floor plate functions to guide spinal cord commissural axons

across the ventral midline (Charron et al., 2003), and Shh expressed at the chiasm functions as a regulator of retinal ganglion cell growth cone extension (Trousse et al., 2001). The Shh-dependent guidance of commissural axons in the spinal cord appears to require the Shh coreceptor Boc (Okada et al., 2006), but does not require Gli transcriptional activation (Yam et al., 2009). Shh expression has also been observed in both the juvenile and adult cerebral cortex (Charytoniuk et al., 2002) outside of known progenitor zones. Recently Shh expression has also been identified in cortical pyramidal neurons (Garcia et al., 2010). However, the function Shh in cortical neurons and the type of neurons expressing Shh remained unknown.

, 1999) For example, studies have shown that

, 1999). For example, studies have shown that Forskolin molecular weight the representation of direction in the caudate preceded in time the representation in PFC early in learning and perhaps served as a teaching signal for the PFC (Antzoulatos and Miller, 2011 and Pasupathy and Miller, 2005).

This is generally consistent with our finding that the caudate had an enriched representation of value derived from the reinforcement learning algorithm in the fixed condition. The learning in Pasupathy and Miller, however, evolved over about 60 trials, whereas the selection in our task evolved over 3–4 trials, making it difficult for us to examine changes in the relative timing of movement signals with learning, to compare our results directly. Much of the work that suggests a role for the striatum in RL has been motivated by the strong projection of the midbrain dopamine

neurons to the striatum (Haber et al., 2000) and the finding that dopamine neurons signal reward prediction errors (Schultz, 2006). Evidence also suggests, however, that dopamine neurons can be driven by aversive events (Joshua et al., 2008, Matsumoto and Hikosaka, 2009 and Seamans Capmatinib and Robbins, 2010), and therefore a straightforward interpretation of dopamine responses as a reward prediction error is not possible. It is still possible that striatal neurons represent action value. Although this has been shown previously (Samejima et al., 2005), similar value representations have been seen in the cortex (Barraclough et al., 2004, Kennerley and Wallis, 2009, Leon and Shadlen, 1999 and Platt and Glimcher, 1999), and therefore the specific role of the striatal action value signal was unclear. As we recorded from both lPFC and the dSTR simultaneously, we were able to show that there was an enrichment of value representations in the dSTR relative to the lPFC in the same task. Interestingly, this was true in both the random and fixed task conditions. In the fixed task condition we found that activity scaled with a value estimate from a reinforcement learning algorithm, and in the

tuclazepam random and fixed conditions the activity scaled with the color bias, which is related to the animals’ probability of advancing in the sequence and ultimately the number of steps necessary to get the reward. This finding is consistent with a role for the dSTR in reinforcement learning, although it suggests a more general role in value representation, as the neurons represent value in both random and fixed conditions. The representation in the random condition is consistent with finding from previous studies (Ding and Gold, 2010). One interesting question is where the action value information comes from, if not from lPFC. There are three likely candidates. One is the dopamine neurons, which have a strong projection to the striatum (Haber et al., 2000) and respond to rewards and reward prediction errors (Joshua et al.